Music and the Brain Our brains have only so much room. We do not have the space for
separate "processors" for each topic we think about. As the
French composer Maurice Ravel (1875-1937) discovered, music and language
seem to be next-door neighbors in Wernicke's area, which lies behind the
auditory area in the left hemisphere (Sergent, J., 1993). In 1933, Ravel started making spelling errors on manuscripts. By the
end of the year he could not read, and could not write his name, thus
showing signs of Wernicke's aphasia. At the same time, he lost his ability
to write down his music. One of his friends heard him say, "... this
opera is here, in my head. I hear it, but I will never write it... I can
no longer write my music." He also could not sight-read music. Yet
Ravel still could play scales, and a few pieces from memory, and he could
tell when a performer missed a note when playing his pieces. He still knew
music, but he could not translate music into written form. Even though
Ravel learned music and language at different times, the processes he used
to translate both words and sounds into writing seem to have taken place
in the same part of the brain. Motion Perception in Space It's hard to keep a sense of balance in space. Astronauts in low
gravity often feel themselves moving and tumbling when they are sitting
still. Worse yet, half of them suffer motion sickness for two to four days
at the start of their flights (Newberg, 1994). Researchers have guessed
that the astronauts get sick because the information coming from their
vestibular systems and their visual systems does not agree; astronauts
often see themselves moving without "feeling" the motion. French scientists Cauquil et al. (1997) tested people on earth with
astronaut-style sensory conflicts. The researchers asked subjects to stand
and try to balance on a pivoting board. One group put on a pair of glasses
that rotated images by moving prisms in the lenses. Another group wore
electrodes that stimulated their vestibular systems with very weak pulses
of electricity (the subjects could not feel the shock). All of the
subjects were worse at balancing when they were being stimulated. The
pulses made 57% of the subjects sick, while the prism glasses gave 73% of
subjects symptoms. When the vestibular-pulse subjects closed their eyes
during the stimulation, though, their symptoms became much less severe,
showing that it really was visual-vestibular conflict that made them feel
sick, and not some other disturbance. |