|
 |  Learning: Principles and Applications, 4/e Stephen B Klein,
Mississippi State University
Memory Storage
Chapter Outline
Chapter Outline
MEASURES OF MEMORY
There are two explicit measures of memory:
recall and recognition. Recall
requires the subject to generate the details of a
memory, similar to an essay exam. Free
recall involves generating the details without
any specific directions or cues such as the request
to name the fifty United States. In cued
recall, information is presented to assist in
the generation of the memory; instead of simply
requesting that all fifty states be named provide
the first letter of each state. It should be noted
that in any exam a cue is presented to assist in
memory retrieval, this cue is the question itself.
Recognition measures the ability to identify
information that had been experienced at an earlier
time. A multiple-choice exam is analogous to
recognition as a measure of memory. The student is
presented with a question and asked to recognize
the correct answer.
There are at least two implicit measures of
memory: savings and reaction time.
Created by Ebbinghaus (1885), savings refers to the
fact that fewer trials (savings) are required to
"re-learn" material, implying the existence of some
residual memory from original learning. When
required to retrieve stored information, the amount
of time necessary (reaction time) to adequately
access the information varies with the
accessibility of the memory; faster reaction times
are associated with stronger memory traces.
A THREE-STAGE VIEW OF MEMORY
According to Atkinson and Shiffrin (1971) memory consists
of three interrelated systems: sensory store (also known as
the sensory register), short-term store, and long-term
store.
The sensory store holds external input for a brief
period of time, usually 500 msec up to 1-2 sec.
This information is an initial impression of the
external stimulus and decays rapidly unless it
undergoes further processing.
The short-term store is the second stage of
information processing and is a temporary,
limited-capacity facility for our experiences. The
length of time information remains in the
short-term store depends on whether it undergoes
rehearsal or not. Without rehearsal information is
lost after approximately 15-30 sec. New incoming
information may displace older information if the
capacity of the short-term store is exceeded. An
important role played by this system is the
organization of material for meaningful storage and
subsequent retrieval.
The long-term store is presumed to be an
unlimited, relatively permanent storage system.
Information stored in this system may or may not be
easily accessed. The ease with which the material
is accessed correctly depends on the degree of
interference from other memories and the presence
of memory attributes.
SENSORY REGISTER
Research on the sensory stores has focused on two sensory
systems: the visual system and the auditory system. The
visual representation in sensory memory is termed an
icon and visual sensory memory is known as iconic
memory. The auditory sensory experience is called an
echo and auditory sensory memory is known as
echoic memory.
Iconic Memory
In Search of an Icon
Sperling (1960, 1963) has examined the
nature of iconic memory. He visually
presented to subjects a matrix of letters
for a brief period of time and then asked
for recall of the matrix. This procedure,
known as the whole-report technique,
indicated that subjects reported on average
4.5 items from the matrices that had as
many as 12 items originally. Sperling
conducted another version of the
experiment, using the partial-report
technique. In this version, the
subjects are asked to recall information
presented in one of three rows. A tone
given at the end of the retention interval
indicated which row had to be recalled.
Each row contained four items and Sperling
discovered that the subjects could remember
most of the letters. However, if the
retention interval was greater than 0.25
sec, subject performance declined to the
level that only one item could be
remembered in each cued row. Sperling's
results suggest that the stored icon is
subject to a rapid decay process.
Duration of an Icon
Sperling's (1960) original research
suggested that icons last for 0.25 sec.
However, as the intensity of the visual
stimulus increases so does the duration of
the icon, at least up to 1 sec or more (if
the display is very dark following
presentation of the letters). In addition,
if the display is very bright before or
after matrix presentation the icon may last
less than 0.25 sec.
Echoic Memory
In Search of an Echo
Neisser (1967) called the sensory
memory of an auditory stimulus an echo.
Moray, Bates, and Barnett (1965) conducted
research indicating that the properties of
echoic memory are similar to those
for iconic memory.
Duration of an Echo
The duration of an echo is longer than an
icon, extending to 2 sec. Research by
Crowder and Morton used the serial
position effect to demonstrate the
longer duration of echoic memory.
The Nature of Sensory Memory
Reseachers initially thought that a sensory memory
was an exact replica of an event; that is, an icon
is an exact copy of a visual stimulus and an echo
is an exact duplicate of a sound (Posner, 1982).
More recent evidence reveals that some processing
occurs in the sensory store, in particular some
form of categorization. Although auditory and
visual sensory memories are important, it should be
noted that sensory memories exist for all the
senses.
SHORT-TERM STORE
The short-term store receives information from the sensory
stores but unlike the sensory registers, the short-term
store actively processes the information. The short-term
store holds information by rehearsal and its function is to
organize and analyze information. It has a limited capacity
and is susceptible to disruption when rehearsal is
interrupted. The function of rehearsal is to increase the
recall of information by allowing the information to enter
the long-term store.
The Span of Short-Term Memory
Peterson and Peterson's (1959) study suggested
that short-term memory rapidly loses information
when it is prevented from being rehearsed.
Disrupting Short-Term Memory
The short-term store has limited capacity but is
required to constantly process incoming
information. Therefore, it is susceptible to
disruption from external and internal sources.
Unless the information is stored in a meaningful
way in the long-term store it will be lost forever.
Limited Storage Capacity
Miller (1956) proposed that the short-term store
holds on average 7 items of information plus or
minus 2 (5 to 9 items). Watkins (1974) proposed
that the actual capacity of the short-term store is
about 3-4 items based on the idea that some of the
items represent information retrieved from the
long-term store.
The Organization of Our Experiences
One type of organization that the short-term store
accomplishes is chunking. A second type is
coding and a third type is the formation of
associations.
Chunking
Chunking involves combining individual
elements into meaningful units of
information. It is an automatic
organizational process of the short-term
store and significantly enhances our
ability to recall past experiences.
Chunking also causes the information
to be recalled from the long-term store in
a specific order based on categories. The
recall of information in terms of
categories is called clustering.
Coding
The coding of information involves
transforming information into a totally new
form. Visual experiences are also coded in
terms of an acoustic code presumably
to enhance the thinking process.
Information is also coded as a visual
code. Some individuals are able to
generate a very distinct visual image of a
past event, a visual memory known as an
eidetic image. A vivid, detailed, and
long-lasting memory for an emotionally
arousing event is called a flashbulb
memory.
Association of Events
Collins and Quillian suggest that semantic
memory consists of interconnected concepts
and properties associated with each concept
node. This perspective is known as the
hierarchical approach. Spreading
activation theory assumes that a
property may be associated with more than
one concept but will be more closely
associated with some concepts. Once a
concept is activated, the activation
spreads to associated concepts or
properties, a perspective that explains the
effect of priming. The notion that
knowledge is not localized but rather is
distributed throughout the entire memory
system is called the parallel processing
model.
The Rehearsal Function of the Short-Term Store
Rehearsal serves two functions (Eich, 1985).
First, it retains information in the short-term
store so it is not forgotten and second, it
provides the opportunity to make the information
more meaningful. The first function corresponds to
maintenance rehearsal which involves simply
repeating the information until it is needed. The
second function is accomplished by elaborative
rehearsal which involves embellishing the
material to create a more meaningful memory.
ALTERNATIVES TO THE ATKINSON-SHIFFRIN MULTI-STAGE MODEL
The Atkinson-Shiffrin model assumes that while in the
short-term store, the memory is rehearsed, which results in
organization and an increase in the likelihood it will be
retrieved later. Two alternatives to this position have
been proposed. One, the rehearsal systems approach
is a modification of the Atkinson-Shiffrin model of the
short-term store and the second, levels of
processing dispenses with the stage notion altogether.
Rehearsal Systems Approach
The rehearsal systems approach, developed by
Baddeley (1990) argues that while memories are
retained in the sensory systems, those memories are
acted on by a process he called working
memory. Working memory retains all the
characteristics of the short-term store but also
contains several rehearsal systems: the
phonological loop for auditory information, the
visuospatial sketch pad for imaging, and a
central executive to coordinate the
activities of the rehearsal systems.
Levels of Processing Approach
The levels of processing view posed by Craik and
Lockhart (1972) assumes that a memory can be
processed at different levels. Information can
receive only a shallow, superficial analysis or it
can be interpreted at a deeper, semantic level. The
deeper or more elaborate the information
processing, the more likely it is that it will be
recalled later.
LONG-TERM STORE
The long-term store preserves information on a relatively
permanent basis. There are several different types of
long-term memories including episodic, semantic,
declarative, and procedural. The latter two are
posited by those investigating the physiological bases of
memory formation and forgetting.
Episodic vs. Semantic Memories
Tulving suggested a distinction between episodic
and semantic long-term memories. An episodic memory
consists of information about events and their
associated time and place. Semantic memory contains
knowledge associated with the use of language
(meanings expressed through the use of symbols).
Types of Information
Separate Operations
The episodic memory system holds sensory
experiences; however, the semantic memory
system records knowledge conveyed by
language. The temporal order of events can
be detected by episodic memory in contrast
to semantic memory which must infer
temporal sequencing. Recollection from
episodic memory is deliberate and often
requires conscious effort whereas recall
from semantic memory is often unconscious
and automatic. Thus, humans remember
episodic information but know
semantic information.
Two Functionally Different Memory Systems?
Although rCBF studies support Tulving's
notion of separate long-term systems,
several researchers disagree and posit a
single memory system with content that can
vary from highly context-specific episodes
to abstract generalizations.
Procedural versus Declarative Memories
Squire has proposed the term procedural
memory for knowledge and recall of motor
skills. Procedural memories are acquired slowly
through repeated experience and we tend to be
unaware of them. In contrast, declarative
memory represents factual memory which is
conscious and can exist as a verbal thought or
non-verbal image.
In Search of the Engram
The engram represents the physiological correlate
of long-term memory. An engram may be the result of
cellular modification, a change in neural
responsiveness due to structural changes at the
synapse, chemical enhancement, or the establishment
of new connections.
Learning in the Aplysia californica
Kandel and his associates (1991, 1993)
have conducted extensive research
indicating that modification of
neurotransmitter function is important for
memory formation. Kandel's research
involves the investigation of habituation,
sensitization, and classical conditioning
of the gill reflex in the sea mollusk
Aplysia californica.
Structural Changes and Experience
Lynch has proposed that structural
modifications of receptor sites on
dendritic spines underlies memory
formation. Presumably, neural activity
allows an enzyme, calpain to expose more
receptor surfaces for neurotransmitter
activity. Calpain's breakdown of the
cellular coating allows dendrites to change
shape and spread out resulting in new
neural connections.
Anatomical Basis of Memory Formation
At least two brain structures - the medial
temporal lobe and the mediodorsal
thalamus appear to be important areas for the
storage of information. Information is initially
processed in the cortex and is then sent down to
the medial temporal lobe and associated structures
(e.g., hippocampus) which project to the
mediodorsal thalamus.
Medial Temporal Lobe
The case of H.M. reveals the importance of
an intact medial temporal lobe including
the hippocampus. To control epilepsy a
bilateral temporal lobectomy was performed
which also removed the hippocampi.
Following the operation, H.M. developed
anterograde amnesia, the inability to
form new memories. This amnesia affected
new declarative memories but not new
procedural memories. The specific cause of
H.M.'s amnesia appears to be due to the
bilateral removal of the hippocampus. Other
clinical cases support this view.
Mediodorsal Thalamus
Damage to or the destruction of the
mediodorsal thalamus also results in
anterograde amnesia for declarative
memories but not as severe as that produced
by destruction of the hippocampus. The
memory deficit called Korsakoff's
syndrome is also associated with damage
to the mediodorsal thalamus.
Mnemonics
People with exceptional memories often use
mnemonic techniques to enable themselves to
recall impressive amounts of recently learned
information. Two commonly used mnemonics are the
method of loci and the pegword
method. In the method of loci the
"to-be-remembered" information is associated with
familiar locations using visual imagery. The
pegword method is effective for remembering the
items on a list; each item is associated with an
already memorized "pegword."
|
|
|