 |  Learning: Principles and Applications, 4/e Stephen B Klein,
Mississippi State University
The Modification Of Instinctive Behavior
Chapter Outline
Chapter Outline
THE INSTINCTIVE BASIS OF BEHAVIOR
Lorenz proposed that instinctive systems promote adaptation
to the environment. Moreover, the ability to learn is
programmed into species by their evolutionary histories.
Therefore, the ability to learn is an innate
characteristic.
The Search for Knowledge: Lorenz emphasized that
organisms are "built" through evolution to
process environmental information that is important
for their survival.
The Interaction of Energy and Environment: Lorenz
and his colleague Tinbergen spent many years
observing animal behavior in natural habitats. From
these observations, Lorenz developed an energy
model of instinctive behavior.
-
Energy Model
Lorenz proposed action-specific
energy as the internal force that
accumulates within the organism and
provides the motivation for behavior. It is
action-specific in the sense that each
behavior (action) requires its own specific
energy. As the energy builds, it is
prevented from being released by an
innate releasing mechanism (IRM). The
increasing pressure stimulates
appetitive behavior and energizes
overall behavior, increasing the likelihood
that an animal will encounter an
environment that contains a sign
stimulus. This stimulus is a "trigger"
that releases the accumulated energy. The
result is a fixed action pattern, which
might be considered an instinctive
behavior.
-
Environmental Release
The releaser (sign stimulus) for a fixed
action pattern may be simple or
complex. The probability that a fixed
action pattern will occur depends upon
several factors, including (1) the
intensity of the sign stimulus and (2) the
accumulated level of action-specific
energy.
-
Hierarchical System
Although some simple behaviors involve only
a few movements, many "higher-level"
instincts, such as reproduction, require
many fixed action patterns. In such cases,
Lorenz assumed that energy, in the form of
neural/hormonal events, builds up at this
high level, and is released to lower
levels. The specific fixed action pattern
that occurs at any moment depends upon
which sign stimulus is present. Presumably,
separate brain areas control each action.
Thus, complicated sequences of behaviors,
such as in reproduction, are controlled by
both brain and environmental events in a
coordinated fashion.
-
Conflicting Motives
Because separate fixed action patterns have
their own action-specific energy and sign
stimuli, conflicts can arise when two
opposing systems are triggered by the two
sign stimuli. In this case, energy may
spill over and release some other behavior,
called displacement.
The Importance of Experience: Lorenz recognized
that instinctive behavior is not necessarily rigid
and unmodifiable. He suggested that experience
(learning) can alter the form of appetitive
behaviors, and/or the sensitivity of the IRM to the
sign stimulus. Although there are many parallels
between Lorenz's energy model and the way
environmental cues can release behaviors, the model
is not a perfect one of how brain controls
behavior.
HABITUATION AND SENSITIZATION
Habituation and sensitization are two
fundamental learning processes. In each case, animals
change their reactions to a stimulus with repeated
stimulation. Habituation is defined as a decrease in
responsiveness to a stimulus, as is seen in ingestional
neophobia (decreased avoidance of a novel food over
time). Sensitization refers to an increase in reactivity to
the stimulus.
The Nature of Habituation and Sensitization: Groves
and Thompson suggest that habituation involves
decreased reactivity of innate reflexes, so that
with repeated stimulation, the animal's nervous
system reacts less to the stimulus. Conversely,
sensitization may occur because of increasing
nervous activity with repeated exposures to the
particular stimulus and perhaps to all stimuli.
The Conditions Affecting Habituation and
Sensitization: Not only are habituation and
sensitization different behaviorally, they differ
in how various conditions affect them. Some of
these factors include (1) the intensity of the
stimulus and (2) the consistency of the stimulus
(e.g., whether its shape changes from stimulation
to stimulation). Both habituation and sensitization
can be transient phenomena. If habituation lasts
over a long time, it suggests some learning has
occurred. Sensitization appears to result from an
increase in overall arousal level.
Dishabituation: Support for increased arousal as a
cause of sensitization is the phenomenon of
dishabituation, defined as the recovery of an
habituated response as a result of encountering a
sensitizing stimulus. Thus, even though an animal
has habituated to a stimulus, some other arousing
stimulus can reinstate the response to the
habituated stimulus. It is adaptive for our nervous
systems to habituate to irrelevant stimulation. It
is also adaptive to be more alert to our
environment when some sensitizing (arousing)
stimulus appears.
OPPONENT-PROCESS THEORY
Solomon and Corbit proposed an opponent-process
theory of emotion to describe how we react to emotional
stimuli, and how our responses change with repeated
exposure to the stimuli.
Our Initial Reaction: An emotional stimulus can
elicit a primary emotional response (A
state), which then instigates an opposite
emotional response (B state). The A state
may be pleasant (thus evoking an unpleasant B
state), or it may be unpleasant (thus evoking a
pleasant B state). The intensity of A is tied to
the intensity of the stimulus, and a given stimulus
will always produce the same A state. The intensity
of B is determined by the intensity of A. Compared
to the A state, B is initially less intense than A,
is sluggish in intensifying, and is also sluggish
in terminating. The overall emotional response to a
stimulus is the summation of A and B over time.
The Intensification of the Opponent B State: The
theory suggests that repeated stimulation (and thus
repeated A states) causes changes in B that include
its (1) more rapid onset, (2) greater intensity,
and (3) slower offset. This may help explain
tolerance and withdrawal in drug
addiction. Tolerance involves reduced reactivity to
a drug with repeated exposure to the drug.
Withdrawal refers to the adjustment of the body and
a drug craving when the drug is not present. The
development of tolerance is the weakening of the A
state (because B increases). Withdrawal is the
intensification of the B state, especially when no
drug is present.
The Addictive Process: Addiction does not always
develop in people who use drugs. But when it does,
Solomon says it is because addicts recognize that
drug abstinence produces withdrawal symptoms (B
state). Thus addicts seek the drug (which increases
the A state) simply to overcome the unpleasant
feelings of withdrawal.
The Influence of Other Aversive Events: The theory
may help explain why addictions are difficult to
cure. The addict's B state is a generalized
discomfort. Thus, any other situation that arouses
a similar general discomfort might motivate the
addict to seek out the drug again to decrease the
discomfort.
The Search for Pleasure: Addiction to most drugs is
based on the drug producing a pleasant A state,
which is followed by an unpleasant B (withdrawal)
state. But some behaviors, such as thrill-seeking,
may represent a situation that produces an
unpleasant A state with a pleasant B state that
intensifies with repeated stimulation.
|
|