 |  Learning: Principles and Applications, 4/e Stephen B Klein,
Mississippi State University
Stimulus Control Of Behavior
Chapter Outline
Chapter Outline
THE GENERALIZATION PROCESS
Generalization is the process that allows responding to
stimuli that are similar to the cues used in original
training. Discrimination is the process of responding to
some stimuli but not others. Generalization and
discrimination are studied by plotting a generalization
gradient which shows the relationship between the degree of
change in the original conditioning stimulus and response
strength.
Generalization Gradients: Generalization gradients
have been plotted for stimuli based upon excitatory
conditioning. There is also a limited amount of
research studying inhibitory generalization
gradients.
Excitatory-Conditioning Generalization
Gradients
Much of the research on excitatory
generalization gradients has used pigeons
as subjects. Guttman and Kalish (1956)
performed the classic excitatory
generalization experiment. They trained
different groups of pigeons to peck a
stimulus key that varied in its wavelength.
After training, each group was given test
stimuli, including wavelengths that were
higher and lower than the original
stimulus. Response levels were measured for
each test stimulus. Guttman and Kalish
consistently observed symmetrical
generalization gradients with the highest
level of responding occurring at the
original training stimulus.
The same type of excitatory generalization
gradients found by Guttman and Kalish are
common in other species, including humans.
Flat generalization gradients can be
observed under certain circumstances.
Inhibitory-Conditioning Generalization
Gradients
Weisman and Palmer (1969) studied
inhibitory generalization gradients.
Pigeons were first trained to peck a green
disk (S+) to receive food reinforcement.
When a white vertical line (S-) was
presented, the pigeons were not reinforced
for key pecking. Inhibitory generalization
tests were conducted with other lines that
differed from the original vertical line by
various degrees of arc. The results showed
the greater the difference between the
original vertical line and the test line,
the less was key pecking inhibited.
The Nature of the Generalization Process: The
Lashley-Wade theory of stimulus generalization
proposes that generalization occurs when the
subject cannot distinguish between the test
stimulus and the conditioning stimulus. Several
lines of research support this explanation.
DISCRIMINATION LEARNING
Discrimination learning involves the use of environmental
cues to signal the availability or unavailability of
positive and negative reinforcement as well as punishment.
In discrimination training, the symbol SD is used to
indicate the availability of reinforcement contingent upon
the occurrence of an appropriate response. The symbol S Δ is
used to indicate that reinforcement is not available and a
response will not be effective in producing reinforcement.
When operant behavior is under the control of a
discriminative stimulus (either SD or SΔ ), Skinner called
the behavior a discriminative operant.
Discrimination Paradigms
Two-Choice Discrimination Tasks
In the two-choice discrimination learning
situation, the SD and the SΔ are within the
same stimulus dimension. In this task,
subjects usually begin by responding
equally to the SD and the SΔ . However, with
continued training, the response rate to
the SD increases and the response rate to
the SΔ decreases.
Conditional Discrimination Task
In a conditional discrimination task, the
availability of reinforcement to a
particular stimulus depends upon the
presence of a second stimulus.
Behavioral Contrast: In discrimination tasks, the
increased responding to the SD and decreased
responding to the SΔ is called behavioral contrast.
Flaherty suggested that two types of contrast
occur. Local contrast is activated by the sudden
change in stimulation (SD to SΔ or vice versa) and
is due to a temporary emotional change (elation or
frustration). Sustained contrast (sometimes called
anticipatory contrast) occurs when animals
anticipate an impending change to more or less
preferred reinforcement contingencies.
Occasion Setting
The Occasion-Setting Function of a
Conditioned Stimulus
The ability of a CS to enhance responding
to a second stimulus is called occasion
setting because it can facilitate the
effectiveness of the second stimulus's
ability to elicit a conditioned response.
This facilitation effect (occasion setting)
apparently is not due to the excitatory
properties of the CS. That is, it is not
due simply to the ability of the occasion
setter to elicit a conditioned response.
Instead, the facilitating or
occasion-setting property of the CS is
separate from conditioned excitation. To
have a stimulus function as an occasion
setter, it must precede a second CS that is
then paired with an UCS.
The Occasion-Setting Function of a
Discriminative Stimulus
Rescorla and colleagues (1988) have shown
that an occasion setting CS can raise the
level of operant responding. Moreover,
Pavlovian occasion setters and operant
discriminative stimuli are interchangeable,
suggesting a common mechanism of action.
Conditioned Stimuli and
Operant/Instrumental Behavior
CSs appear to exert mediational control
over instrumental performance. Rescorla and
Solomon (1967) proposed that the
mediational effects of CSs are tied to
their ability to arouse central
motivational states. Research has indicated
that a CS associated with food arouses the
appetitive motivational state but inhibits
the aversive motivational state. Also, a CS
associated with a painful event arouses the
aversive motivational state while
inhibiting the appetitive motivational
state. Additionally, a CS predicting the
absence of a reward inhibits the appetitive
motivational state but activates the
aversive motivational state. Finally, a CS
predicting the absence of a painful event
inhibits the aversive motivational state
but excites the appetitive motivational
state.
The Nature of Discrimination Learning: How do we
discriminate the SD from the SΔ ?
The Hull-Spence Theory
The Hull-Spence theory of discrimination
learning provides an associative
description of some essential features of
discrimination learning.
Development of Conditioned Excitation and
Inhibition
According to the Hull-Spence view,
discrimination learning develops in three
stages. First, conditioned excitation
develops to the SD as the result of
reinforcement. Second, conditioned
inhibition develops to the SΔ as the result
of non-reinforcement. Finally, conditioned
excitation and inhibition have
generalization gradients. The algebraic
summation of excitatory and inhibitory
strength determines the response rate to
test stimuli.
The Peak Shift Phenomenon
The Hull-Spence theory predicts the peak
shift phenomenon which refers to the
development of maximum responding to a
stimulus other than the SD that is in a
direction opposite to the SΔ . Experimental
support exists for the peak shift effect.
The Aversive Character of SΔ
Terrace (1964) claims that exposure to the
SΔ produces an unpleasant frustration
emotion that has the effect of increasing
the response rate to other stimuli.
The Transposition Effect
Kohler developed an alternative view to the
Hull-Spence theory. According to Kohler,
stimuli are evaluated in relative, not
absolute, terms. Kohler's view predicts the
transposition effect which proposes that
subjects learn the relationships between
stimuli in discrimination training.
Kohler's relational view is supported in
discrimination tasks in which the subject
must choose between two presented stimuli.
However, the Hull-Spence theory is an
adequate explanation when subjects are
tested with only one stimulus at a time.
Errorless Discrimination Training
Under certain experimental arrangements, a
discrimination can be learned with few or
no errors. a result known as errorless
discrimination learning.
Training Procedure
Errorless discrimination training was
demonstrated by Terrace (1963) whose
pigeons pecked at a red illuminated key
(SD) to receive food reinforcement. When
the pigeons received early and progressive
introduction to the SΔ (a dark key lasting
a short period of time), they learned to
ignore the SΔ even when it reached its full
effect.
Application: Errorless Discrimination
Training in Humans
Errorless discrimination training has also
been observed with humans using the fading
technique.
Nonaversive SΔ
The behavioral characteristics found with
standard discrimination training are not
observed with errorless discrimination
training. The peak shift phenomenon is not
found in errorless discrimination training
suggesting that the SΔ has not acquired
aversive properties.
Sutherland and Mackintosh's Attentional
View
Sutherland and Mackintosh developed an
attentional view of discrimination training
that operates in two stages. In the first
stage, the subject's attention to the
relevant stimulus dimension is
strengthened. In the second stage, the
relevant stimulus is associated with a
response.
The Recognition of the Relevant Dimension
The Sutherland-Mackintosh attentional
theory claims that each stimulus can
activate an analyzer that actively scans
the environment searching for the relevant
aspects of a stimulus configuration. Once a
particular analyzer has been aroused, the
output from the analyzer is associated to a
particular response.
Predictive Value of Discriminative Stimuli
Results by Wagner and associates (1968)
suggest that the relative predictiveness of
the SD, not the percentage of trials on
which the SD is associated with
reinforcement, determines its ability to
control behavior.
Continuity versus Discontinuity
The Hull-Spence view claims that excitation
and inhibition gradually increase during
the acquisition of a discrimination. This
position is called the continuity theory of
discrimination learning. Krechevsky (1932)
and Lashley (1929) presented an alternative
view that denies that discrimination
learning is gradually acquired. Instead,
subjects are presumed to form hypotheses
about the relevant dimension that predicts
reinforcement. The Krechevsky and Lashley
view predicts that discrimination
performance is exceptional once the
relevant stimulus dimension is discovered.
Thus, their view has been referred to the
noncontinuity theory of discrimination
learning.
|
|