 |  Learning: Principles and Applications, 4/e Stephen B Klein,
Mississippi State University
Cognitive Control Of Behavior
Chapter Outline
Chapter Outline
TOLMAN'S PURPOSIVE BEHAVIORISM
Tolman developed a cognitive view of learning during the
era when Hull's mechanistic theory was dominant. However,
Tolman's approach has become increasingly popular in modern
psychology.
Learning Principles: Tolman challenged Hull's claim
that behavior is automatic by stating that behavior
has direction and purpose. Organisms act for ends,
as if behavior were purposeful. Expectations occur
when sufficient motivation is present. Tolman
undertook a series of studies to demonstrate the
purposive nature of behavior.
Place-Learning Studies: Tolman said that subjects
learn the locations of rewards while Hull proposed
that responses are connected to stimuli by
automatic associative processes. Tolman and
associates undertook a series of studies to
investigate the nature of learning.
T-Maze Experiments
Tolman, Ritchie, and Kalish (1946)
completed a study comparing perceptual
versus motor learning of a food location in
a T-maze. The results showed that
perceptual (place) learning was superior to
motor (response) learning.
Numerous studies have since examined place
versus response learning. The degree of
experience with the task appears to be an
important factor. As behavior is
continually practiced, mechanical habit
processes appear to dominate over cognitive
processes in controlling performance.
Alternate-Path Studies
Tolman and Honzik (1930) studied rats in a
maze where familiar pathways to the goal
were blocked. The rats were adaptive in
changing their behavior for the positive
reinforcement as successive pathways were
blocked. Tolman interpreted these results
to mean that rats learn a perceptual
representation of the environment in order
to locate reward. However, other research
has indicated that changing the width of
the path in the maze leads to the animal
moving down the blocked path.
Is Reward Necessary for Learning?: Tolman stated
that contiguity of two events was sufficient for
learning to occur. His latent-learning experiments
were intended to verify this claim.
Latent-Learning Studies
Tolman and Honzik's (1930) classic
experiment indicated that the introduction
of food reinforcement after hungry rats had
experienced a maze is followed by immediate
improvement in behavior. This improvement
indicated to Tolman and Honzik that
learning had previously taken place in the
absence of reinforcement. When the
reinforcement was introduced, what was
previously learned began to influence
behavior.
While Tolman and Honzik's suggest that
latent learning can occur, other studies
have not been able to confirm the effect.
One important factor in determining whether
latent learning can be demonstrated is the
drive state of the subject. As the drive
state increases, the probability of latent
learning decreases.
The Drive Response
Latent learning effects are most
consistently observed when animals are not
deprived when initially exposed to reward.
In addition, it is possible that unintended
sources of reward are operating in latent
learning experiments.
THE CONCEPT OF AN EXPECTANCY
A Mental Representation of Events: Several
psychologists have argued that an expectancy is a
mental representation of events.
Types of Mental Representations: Dickinson proposed
two main types of expectancies. First, there is an
associative-linkexpectancy which allows one event
to excite or inhibit the representation of another
event. Secondly, there is a behavior-belief
representation which is knowledge that a specific
action will be followed by a certain consequence.
Associative-Link Expectancies: According to
Dickinson, Pavlovian conditioning results in
associative-link expectancies. Associative-link
expectancy is demonstrated by the irrelevant
incentive effect which involves the acquisition of
an excitatory link expectancy that a particular
stimulus is associated with a reinforcer when an
irrelevant drive state is active.
Behavior-Reinforcer Beliefs: Dickinson also
proposed that operant behavior is controlled by
behavior-reinforcer beliefs. Dickinson says that
the reinforcer devaluation effect demonstrates the
existence of behavior-reinforcer beliefs. When a
positive reinforcer is associated with an aversive
event, the positive reinforcer loses its ability to
control behavior.
The Importance of Habits: Some theorists have
rejected the idea that expectancies control
behavior and have returned to the Hullian belief
that mechanical habits control performance.
Dickinson has tried to establish the conditions
under which habits might dominate behavior. As
behavior continues to be practiced, it appears to
come under the stronger influence of habit, a
phenomenon Dickinson calls behavioral autonomy.
A COGNITIVE VIEW OF DEPRESSION
Seligman (1975) has offered a learned helplessness theory
of depression. The theory suggests that learning plays an
important role in emotions.
Learned Helplessness Theory: Learned helplessness,
as an explanation for depression, proposes that
exposure to uncontrollable events leads to the
belief that events are independent of behavior.
Original Animal Research
Seligman and associates trained dogs in an
inescapable shock condition. Later, these
dogs had major difficulty learning how to
avoid shock as compared to control
subjects.
Helplessness in Human Subjects Hiroto
(1974) showed that humans who were trained
in an inescapable noise situation later had
difficulty learning how to avoid the noise
stimulus.
Characteristics of Helplessness
According to Seligman, helplessness
produces motivational deficits, cognitive
deficits, and emotional disturbance.
Motivational Impairments
The passivity of subjects following
exposure to uncontrollable events indicates
a loss to initiate voluntary behavior.
Intellectual Impairments
Exposure to uncontrollable events appears
to cause subjects to lose interest in the
learning process. Expectations of future
success or failure of behavior is dependent
upon the subject maintaining control over
the situation.
Emotional Trauma
Exposure to uncontrollable events induces
symptoms associated with clinical
depression.
Similarities of Helplessness and Depression
Correlational evidence supports Seligman's
view that the inability to control events
precipitates depression.
Criticism of the Learned Helplessness
Approach
The original learned helplessness view has
been criticized for its simplicity. Some
studies with humans reported contradictory
outcomes. These problems led to a revision
of the theory in the form of an
attributional model.
An Attributional Model: Seligman and associates
(1978, 1980) proposed that the attributions people
make for failure determine whether they become
depressed. An attribution is a belief, a perceived
cause for an event. Causal attributions operate on
three dimensions: personal-universal
(internal-external), global-specific, and
stable-unstable. A personal attribution is the
belief that internal characteristics are
responsible for the outcome of a situation; an
external attribution is a view that agencies, other
than the self, determine an outcome. A global
attribution is a belief that the cause of an
outcome on a certain task will determine outcomes
in other situations; a specific attribution is a
belief that the cause for an outcome operates
uniquely. Finally, a stable attribution is a belief
that the perceived cause of an outcome will
determine future outcomes; an unstable attribution
is a belief that new factors may determine future
outcomes.
Personal versus Universal Helplessness
The nature of the helplessness determines
whether loss of esteem appears. People who
attribute their failure to external forces
- universal helplessness - experience no
loss of self-esteem. However, the
attribution of failure to internal factors
- personal helplessness - does lead to the
loss of self-esteem and the induction of
depression.
Global versus Specific Causal Attributions
If failure is attributed to a specific
situation, then helplessness does not
occur. Depression results when people feel
that their failure is global.
Stable versus Unstable Causal Attributions
If failure is attributed to an unstable
factor, then the attribution does not lead
to helplessness. However, if failure is
attributed to a stable feature of the
personality, then depression results.
Severity of Depression
Depression is most severe when people
attribute their failures to internal,
global, and stable factors.
Hopelessness: Abramson, Metalsky, and Alloy (1989)
have suggested a further revision of helplessness
theory. They propose that some forms of depression
are caused by hopelessness, which refers to the
expectation either that desired outcomes will not
occur or that the person has no control over
undesired outcomes. This attributional style can
lead to hopelessness depression.
A Pessimistic Explanatory Style and Depression:
Research has indicated that knowing a person's
attributional style allows the prediction whether
the individual is susceptible to depression: An
internal, global, and stable attributional style is
a marker for depression. Langer (1983) proposed
that an optimistic personality style insulates
against depression: Optimistic individuals tend to
feel competent (i.e., believe they have personal
control over external events).
Biological Influences on Learned Helplessness:
Biochemical changes in the brain induced by stress
(e.g., uncontrollable, unpleasant events) may
precipitate depression. For example, decreased
norepinephrine activity correlates with depression.
A COGNITIVE VIEW OF PHOBIC BEHAVIOR
Phobias are unrealistic fears and may be based on cognitive
processes.
Phobias and Expectations: According to Bandura, a
phobia is driven by two classes of expectancies.
First, stimulus-outcome expectancies reflect the
perceived relationships of events. Second,
response-outcome expectancies indicate personal
ability regarding a particular task. These
expectancies can lead to a belief that one can or
cannot succeed, which is termed an efficacy
expectation.
The Importance of Our Experiences: Outcome and
efficacy expectations can develop through direct
personal experience, observations of other people's
experiences, and through information provided by
others. Moreover, one's sense of efficacy can also
be influenced by one's emotions.
Application: A Modeling Treatment for Phobias:
Modeling allows for the acquisition of behavior by
observing others. Thus, modeling may be useful in
the treatment of phobia. In therapy, clients often
reduce their phobias by watching a model move
closer in successive steps until a feared object is
encountered.
An Alternative View: Some psychologists continue to
argue that avoidance behavior is largely
drive-based. Thus, phobic behavior may not always
be best explained by cognitive processes such as
those proposed by Bandura and others.
|
|